Editor's comment

AC Grayling is professor of philosophy at Birkbeck College, University of London. He has written and edited many books on philosophy and other subjects.

 

Reading on: Mirror neurons and morality

– neurological research undermines moral relativism. Morality is hard wired in the brain

Grayling, A. C., Our mirror on morality New Scientist 30 April 2008 [extract - 500 words]

Empirical inquiry can have a major impact on normativebased on an accepted standard considerations, and current research in neurology appears to be doing just this. In particular, it can be applied to one of the most vexing problems in moral philosophy: the problem of relativismmorality is established by society or culture. What neurology reveals about brain function might already have refuted relativism and established the ground for saying that the basis of morality is shared by all humans.
If so, this is a truly major result.

Moral relativism is the view that there are no universal truths about what is right and wrong, but rather that what counts as such in each different society is determined by its own traditions, beliefs and experience. Since these can differ markedly among societies, it follows that different societies can have quite opposite views about what is right. And this, says the relativist, means there is no objective ground for deciding between them.

This view seems compelling when we consider such contrasts as different societies' views about, say, polygamy or homosexuality. The motive for relativism is the worthy one of avoiding the arrogance of cultural imperialism as practised by dominant societies in the past, as they colonised other peoples and imposed moralities upon them. Relativists wish to assert the equal dignity and validity of different societies and their moral outlooks, even when they clash with one's own.

The neurological research that undermines moral relativism concerns the function of mirror neurons in the brain. Located in the motor cortex, mirror neurons activate in sympathy with what their owner perceives in the activity and experience of others. When we see someone else acting in a particular way - smiling, yawning, weeping or grimacing, for example - the neurons associated with these actions in one's own motor cortex fire in response. Their activation provides a model of what that person is experiencing, giving us a form of direct insight into other people's states of mind.

This strongly suggests that the ability to understand others, read their intentions, interpret their emotional states, predict their behaviour and respond appropriately - the very basis of social capacity itself, and thus of morality - is linked to the involuntary modelling of others that mirror-neuron activity makes possible.

Some researchers hypothesise that malfunction in these neurons might be a factor in autism, one of whose major symptoms is the inability to engage socially with others. There are also strong links between mirror neurons and language capacity, humans' chief social tool.
The essential point is that mirror neurons underwrite the ability to recognise what helps or distresses others, what they suffer and enjoy, what they need and what harms them. This means that the ultimate basis for moral judgement is hard-wired - and therefore universal. So even when customs differ, fundamental morality does not; and if two societies differ over what they consider to be moral, one of them must just be plain wrong.